SpeechPathology.com Phone: 800-242-5183


Progressus Therapy

Grammar: "Ready to Use" or "Requires Assembly"

Grammar: "Ready to Use" or "Requires Assembly"
Judith Johnston
January 21, 2010
Share:

My basement flooded last week and in order to improve the drainage I needed to remove my garden shed. As I flipped through a Canadian catalogue looking for a replacement, I noticed that some sheds were virtually ready to use upon delivery while others needed assembly. "Requires two persons and 6-8 hours." It struck me that this distinction in garden sheds has much in common with beliefs about the nature of grammar. There is widespread agreement that adult speakers rely on grammatical representations that include abstract elements such as Noun, Verb, or AUX. Without such abstract categories, we could not continually talk about new things, like the similarity between grammars and garden sheds. But for over 35 years now, there have persisted two very different views of how this abstract adult grammar comes to be.

In the Chomskian view, the human mind is innately provided with a Universal Grammar that includes categories such as Noun and Verb (Chomsky, 1965). Other linguists, however, have not assumed that these categories are available from the outset, and instead have asked how grammatical categories might emerge out of the organizing activities of the human mind (Elman, Bates, Johnson, Kamiloff-Smith, & Parisi, 1996). Is grammar "Ready to Use" or does it "Require Assembly?" Can speech-language patholgists (SLPs) merely open the box, or must we stick around for a while and assist the child in constructing an abstract grammar? It is too soon for a definitive answer, but current research on this topic is intriguing.

Melissa Bowerman (1973) was among the first to caution against assuming too much too soon in children's grammar. She analyzed data from children learning Finnish, and concluded that meaning-based categories such as Agent were closer to the child's competence than meaning-free categories such as Verb. She observed that children's utterances in the early stages were not nearly as productive as a fully abstract system would predict. If children really knew the N + V + N sentence pattern, why was it used with only a small proportion of their verbs? More recently, Michael Tomasello (2000) has taken this vision of an alternative child grammar as the starting point for a new perspective on language learning. He calls it "construction grammar" and it is indeed a theory in which children gradually construct grammatical categories out of instances of particular verbs and sentences.

A Constructivist View of Acquisition

According to Tomasello, language development proceeds through three periods during the preschool years. In the first period, toddlers (under 2:6 or so) combine words using patterns that are lexically specified. Their grammars include virtually none of the high level abstractions such as V, AUX , or PREP, that are key to the adult speaker's language productivity. Instead, toddlers only have one category, Nominals (concrete nouns and pronouns), and their understanding of word combinations is limited to the knowledge that a specific verb can be preceded and/or followed by a Nominal. The resulting grammar, such as it is, consists of patterns such as N + "play" or N + "see" + N. The pattern(s) preferred for a given verb would be the one(s) heard most frequently. Note that grammars of this sort are productive, but to a very limited degree. Each verb is associated with its own specific sentence pattern; only the referents (Nominals) change. The child can put a new noun in the Nominal slot, but there is no further room for creativity.

In the second period, ages 3-4, grammars become more and more abstract. Although the process is not well-understood, new patterns seem to be constructed by analogy. Children see that the relationshipsboth structural and semanticbetween Daddy and fix in the sentence "Daddy fixed the bicycle," are the same as the relationships between Mommy and bake in the sentence "Mommy baked cookies." The construction process is guided by meaning at two levelslexical and combinatorial. For example, when preschoolers construct the transitive sentence pattern N + V + N, they link this ordered string to a causal meaning. Because of this new level of abstraction, four-year-olds can use a new verb in this transitive pattern without ever having heard it used that way, but only if the verb has a causal meaning.


Judith Johnston



Related Courses

Facilitating First Verbs through Shared Book Reading
Presented by Susan Hendler Lederer, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9735Level: Introductory1 Hour
This course discusses early verb acquisition, choosing first verb targets, and a variety of strategies to facilitate verb learning using children’s picture books as a therapy context.

The Role of Relationships in Early Development: The Connection Between Experiences and Language Capacity
Presented by Alison D. Peak, LCSW, IMH-E
Video
Course: #8727Level: Introductory1 Hour
This course will provide information on the connection between early experiences and early formation of language. Through this framework, the course will look at the impact of Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) and serve-and-return interactions on the language gap that exists for many communities across the US.

Embedding Intervention Strategies into Everyday Activities of Infants/Toddlers and Their Families
Presented by Juliann Woods, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9269Level: Intermediate1 Hour
While embedded intervention is logically appealing and commonly accepted as a recommended practice, implementation often lacks the intentionality and sufficiency necessary to facilitate child progress and caregiver confidence. This course will highlight both intervention and implementation strategies to increase dosage and generalization of learning for young children. This course is presented in partnership with the American Board of Child Language and Language Disorders (ABCLLD).

Shared Book Reading: If You Give a Mouse Some Mindfulness
Presented by Susan Hendler Lederer, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #10818Level: Introductory1 Hour
Shared book reading is a powerful tool for facilitating language and literacy. The evidence supporting mindfulness practices, and methods for integrating them into shared book-reading activities in order to enhance a child’s ability to listen, look, and learn are discussed in this course. Specific books that are appropriate for such therapy activities will be described.

Autism Outreach Podcast: Early Intervention Tips - Embedding Language into Everyday Routines
Presented by Rosemarie Griffin, MA, CCC-SLP, BCBA, Kimberly Scanlon, MA, CCC-SLP
Audio
Course: #9806Level: Introductory0.5 Hours
This podcast addresses ways to embed language early intervention (EI) into functional routines using connection-building, playful activities that don't take away from daily life. Simple strategies that can be used in therapy sessions are discussed.

Our site uses cookies to improve your experience. By using our site, you agree to our Privacy Policy.