SpeechPathology.com Phone: 800-242-5183


The Stepping Stones Group - Opportunities - November 2023

Research Watch Report, Issue 6

Research Watch Report, Issue 6
Meredith Poore Harold, PhD, CCC-SLP
January 15, 2018
Share:

The following is the September/October 2017 issue of research reviews for pediatric and school-based SLPs, written by Meredith Harold, PhD, CCC-SLP. Each month, Dr. Harold accesses 34 journals with the highest impact ratings for the area (Thompson-Reuters, SCImago, and h5-index). Articles that are (a) relevant to pediatric and school-based SLPs' daily practice, and (b) draw from strong evidence, are included in the monthly review. The research reviews highlight the most important clinical findings, then indicate additional resources to be found in the full article. These reviews are meant to be brief, informative, and—most importantly—keep you up-to-date on evidence-based practice. 

Learning Outcomes

After this course readers will be able to: 

  1. Describe current research on how feedback impacts student learning during speech–language therapy.
  2. Describe the profile of literacy skill attainment in preschoolers with and without hearing loss.
  3. Identify how child and environmental factors impact the successful identification and treatment of children with autism.

EPG for Treatment of Articulation Errors: /r/

Wouldn’t it be great if there was some sort of fancy biofeedback device that could be used for the treatment of articulation disorders? There is! One example is electropalatography (EPG) devices like the SmartPalate (http://completespeech.com/smartpalate/), now commercially available, increasingly affordable by clinics and school districts, and marketed for the correction of speech sound errors (/r/ in particular).

But, do we have evidence to support that EPG devices work for speech? That’s exactly what Hitchcock et al. (2017) attempt to tackle. Five elementary-aged children—all with very low accuracy for /r/, four previously treated for /r/ and one with no treatment history—were provided twice-weekly EPG therapy sessions for two months, using SmartPalate. At the end of treatment, all of the children made some gains with /r/, but it was quite variable, with only some of the children generalizing what they had learned to untreated /r/ targets.

So, what do we do with this information? Well, the good news is that we know it can work for some kids! The bad news is this study is very small, and represents only the beginning of the type of data that needs to be collected in order to make truly informed decisions about the need for such technology. We have no idea how EPG biofeedback fares compared to other types of biofeedback, including ultrasound biofeedback as described by Preston, Brick and Landi in their article, "Ultrasound Biofeedback Treatment for Persisting CAS" (2013) and Byun & Hitchcock in their article, "Investigating the Use of Traditional and Spectral Biofeedback Approaches to Intervention for /r/ Misarticulation" (2012) as well as visual–acoustic biofeedback (seeing visual representations of acoustic features, e.g. on a computer screen). And, realistically, what is best may vary per child characteristics and treatment target—that is, acoustic biofeedback versus tongue shape biofeedback (as in ultrasound) versus tongue-to-palate contact information (as in EPG) could all be more or less ideal depending on what speech sound you are working on. For example, with EPG treatment of /r/, the child is getting feedback about lateral bracing only (what’s lateral bracing? Read "Speech-Language Pathologists' Knowledge of Tongue/Palate Contact for Consonant" (Sharynne Mcleod, 2011; http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.3109/02699206.2011.620678?journalCode=iclp20&). However, “…articulatory imaging has shown that speakers produce English rhotics using a variety of different tongue shapes…”

Hitchcock, E.R., Byun, T.A., Swartz, M., & Lazarus, R. (2017). Efficacy of Electropalatography for Treating Misarticulation of /r/. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0122.

Child and Family Characteristics Influence Screening Results for Children with Autism

We all know that not all screening and evaluation instruments “work” the same for all children (well, maybe not… but we’re at least figuring that out, slowly but surely…). Moody et al. provide us with yet another example, in the screening of young children with autism (ASD).

They examined the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) and Social Responsiveness Scales (SRS) questionnaires across thousands of children, and found that lower maternal education and household income predicted a greater likelihood of the test incorrectly identifying the child as one who may have autism, in both screeners. Also, in children with developmental and behavioral challenges, the researchers found that the screeners picked up on these things, but couldn’t reliably separate them from characteristics of ASD, thus also over-identifying these children. The researchers emphasize that their results do not mean that the SRS and SCQ shouldn’t be used; rather, at this time, clinicians must simply must be aware of which child characteristics are most likely to make certain screeners under-perform. They suggest a “… need to refine existing measures or develop new instruments to ensure they perform well across groups.”

Moody, E.J., Reyes, N., Ledbetter, C., & Wiggins, L. (2017). Screening for Autism with the SRS and SCQ: Variations across Demographic, Developmental and Behavioral Factors in Preschool Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3255-5.

Predictors of Social Success in Children with Autism

“There is a common misconception that all children with ASD experience negative social outcomes and require intense intervention supports in schools. While this may be true for many children with ASD, there are others who are well liked and socially connected.” (Locke et al., 2017)

Many children with autism experience social successes. Why is that? What is it about these kids, about what they’re doing or the environment they’re in, that makes them successful?

First, the authors of this study had to define success. So, what’s a “socially successful” kid? They chose to define it as one who:

  • demonstrates “more joint attention with peers during unstructured school periods (recess and lunch)…”
  • is rated by peers to have a relatively strong role within the classroom social environment; this is kind of like “popularity” except was measured by both individual and peer group popularity

Working with that definition, they looked at 148 kids with autism, ages 5–12, who spent at least half their day in a general education environment. Some things they found:

  1. Over half the children had “notable success on at least one social outcome (so one of the two bullet points, above)
  2. Autism severity impacted social success; IQ didn’t
  3. Social success was easier for the younger kids, and harder for the older kids (and this finding is consistent with other studies, e.g. Rotherman-Fuller et al., 2010); more specifically, being a member of a peer group starts to decrease around age eight.

So the biggest take-homes for SLPs may be to note that many children with autism do have plenty of social success. Not all kids with autism need social support or goals!  Also, older elementary children tend to have less social success than the younger children. This doesn’t exactly tell us what to do with intervention, though. Does knowing that kids tend to have more social difficulties as they get older mean we should treat early to prevent it, or later when it’s really needed? Who knows... but we certainly shouldn't expect to "graduate" kids from their social goals as they age. Unfortunately, the social landscape changes substantially from early to late elementary ages.

What do the authors suggest we do? Again, this isn’t a treatment article, so nothing on treatment can be concluded from this study. But they do point to some options, such as intervention studies on SLPs putting together clubs and social groups (e.g. Kasari et al., 2016).

Note: This article did not appear in one of our regularly-covered journals, but one “outside” of our field.  

Locke, J., Williams, J., Shih, W., Kasari, C. (2017). Characteristics of socially successful elementary school-aged children with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 58(1), 94–102.

Closing the Gap in Reading and Writing Skills of Preschoolers with Hearing Loss

This study compared 19 preschoolers with hearing loss (who used amplification and spoken language) to 14 hearing preschoolers, with the goal of measuring “…change across a 6-month period in emergent literacy skills…”.

The researchers identified a myriad of language, phonological, and print knowledge skill gaps for the preschoolers with hearing loss compared to hearing peers (exception: letter name/sound knowledge… yay alphabet time). However, the growth rate of these skills was similar between groups (e.g. morphosyntax and vocabulary grew at the same rate for both groups). The authors state, “Although children with hearing loss generally demonstrated positive change in emergent literacy skills, their initial performance was lower than that of children with normal hearing, and rates of change were not sufficient to catch up to their peers over time.” Note also that all the kids with hearing loss were receiving speech­–language services—meaning, our business-as-usual may not quite be enough to close the literacy gap for children with hearing loss.

Importantly, this study shows us which of the skills are most likely to be problematic in preschoolers with hearing loss— that is, phonological awareness and print concepts (as tested in the Preschool Print and Word Awareness Test). The authors suggest, “Treatment plans should be developed with these particular difficulties in mind, devoting sufficient resources to scaffold these skills in particular.” And this statement is based on not only their study results, but previous studies as well (see article for review). Nonetheless, this isn’t a treatment study. And we definitely need more treatment studies in order to truly guide us in what needs to be done for preschoolers with hearing loss in order to efficiently close the literacy gap.

Werfel, K.L. (2017). Emergent Literacy Skills in Preschool Children With Hearing Loss Who Use Spoken Language: Initial Findings From the Early Language and Literacy Acquisition (ELLA) Study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48, 249–259.

Throwback (2015): How You Correct Students’ Grammatical Errors Matters

It seems like SLPs generally know what to treat—you treat what’s missing from a child’s speech–language inventory, right? (Well, mostly. Kind of.) However, knowing how to treat is much more challenging. In fact, maybe you haven't even thought of this much yet! This study considers just that, in the context of grammatical intervention for children with developmental language disorder (DLD).

Specifically: How Does Our Response to (or Correction of) Grammatical Errors Impact Learning?

The researchers tested two options for feedback following a child's grammatical error:

  1. Recasting, where the instructor repeats that corrected version (e.g. child says “Mommy coat”; adult responds “Mommy’s coat”)
  2. Cueing hierarchy, where the instructor gradually increases the level of support until the child correctly produces the target (e.g. child says “Mommy coat”; adult asks for clarification, then repeats the child’s incorrect response as a question, then contrasts the correct from incorrect production, then models the correct production and asks the child to say it… there are some really clear examples of this within the study appendix.

Each of these two feedback options were provided by trained SLPs and/or teachers, to five-year-olds with DLD, within a small group setting. After 8 weeks of therapy the winner was……… cueing!

Now, this lends some support to using cueing. But, does it mean we shouldn’t be using recasting? No. We can’t conclude that recasting is ineffective, because other studies have shown that it works within some treatment protocols. Based on the differences between this and other studies’ treatment design, the authors predict, “… recasting treatment may be effective over longer treatment periods.”

So what about cueing made it effective? It provided the student with clearer distinctions between the correct and incorrect form, more attention to correction of these errors, and required production of the correct form. And any or all of these features may contribute to the positive effect.


meredith poore harold

Meredith Poore Harold, PhD, CCC-SLP

Dr. Harold, author of  The Informed SLP, is a Speech–Language Pathologist and Assistant Professor of Communication Sciences and Disorders at Rockhurst University. Her clinical work and scholarship focus on pediatric speech–language development and disorders. 



Related Courses

ApPARENTly This Is Not Going Well: Difficult Conversations with Parents
Presented by Marva Mount, MA, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9726Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This course explores emotional intelligence (EQ) and how to "plug in" and use it in situations that go awry with parents of clients. Specific strategies for handling difficult situations and de-escalating arguments are discussed.

Reading Comprehension and the SLP: Foundational Understanding
Presented by Angie Neal, MS, CCC-SLP
Audio
Course: #10763Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This is Part 1 of a two-part series. This course provides SLPs with foundational knowledge needed to directly address and collaboratively support reading comprehension across all grade levels. Models of language and reading comprehension, comprehension processes vs. products, instruction in comprehension skills vs. strategies, factors in reading comprehension difficulties, and connections to general education are discussed.

Reading Comprehension and the SLP: Contributions of Language
Presented by Angie Neal, MS, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #10764Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This is Part 2 of a two-part series. The connections between reading comprehension and areas of language such as vocabulary, morphosyntax and social communication are described in this course. Implications for intervention/instruction and collaboration with educators are also discussed.

Understanding and Treating Echolalia: When You Means I
Presented by Lisa R. Audet, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #10261Level: Intermediate1 Hour
Echolalic speech can reflect a Gestalt learning style that often accompanies hyperlexia and reading comprehension difficulties. This course provides an explanation of echolalia based on this framework, and describes related characteristics common in children with autism. Intervention strategies related to language, literacy, and socialization are also discussed.

20Q: English Learners and Developmental Language Disorder - ​Strategies to Develop Academic Vocabulary Skills
Presented by Celeste Roseberry-McKibbin, PhD, CCC-SLP, F-ASHA
Text
Course: #10266Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This course discusses Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) in English Learners (EL). Specific, research-based strategies are provided for developing academic vocabulary skills and phonological awareness skills in this group of students.

Our site uses cookies to improve your experience. By using our site, you agree to our Privacy Policy.