SpeechPathology.com Phone: 800-242-5183


Med Travelers - December 2019

Language Impairments & Social-Emotional Communicative Competence

Language Impairments & Social-Emotional Communicative Competence
Carol Westby
July 28, 2008
Share:

 

Introduction

Recent years have witnessed an increased interest in the interrelationships between language and social competence. A great deal of this heightened interest has been triggered by the large numbers of children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) who typically exhibit significant difficulties in pragmaticsthe use of language for social communicative functions. In addition, however, research has shown that children with specific language impairment whose primary deficits are in morphology-syntax areas also exhibit deficits in social uses of language that interfere with their effective functioning in social situations (Brinton & Fujiki, 2000; Fujiki, Brinton, Hart, & Craig, 1999). Deficits in effective social communication have also been identified in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and with behavioral disorders; in children whose primary diagnosis is visual or hearing impairments (Mills 1993; Urwin, 1983); and in children who have experienced early environmental abuse or neglect (Perry, 1997). Pragmatic deficits are recognized in children with ASD and are often a focus of intervention. Pragmatic deficits are less frequently recognized in many other children; and even when they are recognized, they are less likely to be treated. A number of public school speech-language pathologists have reported that they are not permitted to provide treatment for pragmatic or social competence deficits because there is no proof that these problems affect academics.

In actuality, pragmatic deficits can affect students' academic performance in several ways:

  • Teacher-student relationships. Poor pragmatic skills may result in behavioral difficulties that must be addressed by the teacher. As a consequence, a teacher may spend more time addressing a child's behavioral needs and less time addressing the child's academic learning needs.
     
  • Peer relationships. Many classroom activities require that children work in peer groups. Children with poor social communication skills may be less likely to be selected by their peers to participate in group activities. Even when they are part of a group, children with social communicative deficits participate less fully or actively and, as a consequence, have fewer opportunities to learn the expected academic content.
     
  • Ability to make inferences from texts. If children do not understand emotionality and temporal cause-effect relationships in social situations, they will not be able to use such knowledge to build the mental models that are necessary for text comprehension.

This article describes the ways that social-emotional aspects of communication are disrupted in children with a variety of developmental disabilities. Children with cognitive, syntactic, or semantic deficits (for whatever reason) are also likely to show delays and differences in social or pragmatic aspects of communication. Children who were born prematurely, have significant medical problems, or have sensory, linguistic, or cognitive impairments are less able to engage in the conversational dance during infancy, and hence, are at risk for pragmatic deficits beyond what would be expected based on their cognitive abilities alone. A number of conditions may result in the child being less able to be involved in communicative interactions or for the adult to be less able to read the child's involvement.

Blindness

Early social interactions, gesturing, and the development of referencing are all dependent upon vision. Children who are blind are generally delayed in acquisition of first words and frequently exhibit pragmatic language impairments. They repeat words to themselves and fail to produce them to initiate interactions until well into their third year (Urwin, 1983). Later, they may ask many questions, sometimes inappropriately; they may use echolalia and may make "off-the-wall" comments (Mills, 1993). By 6 months of age, the sighted infant has developed a large repertoire of social interactions. Children who are blind, however, have no way of watching their mothers' facial expressions or of engaging in joint attention to visual events with her. And because blind babies stop looking at their mothers after the end of the reflex period, mothers do not pick them up as often. Consequently, infants miss opportunities for communicating.


carol westby

Carol Westby



Related Courses

Neurodiversity: How to Support Agency and Self-determination
Presented by Joleen R. Fernald, PhD, CCC-SLP, BCS-CL
Video
Course: #9590Level: Intermediate1 Hour
It is imperative for clinicians and educators to be aware of the neurodiversity movement in order to better support all human rights. This course discusses what neurodiversity is and the history of the movement, as well as the potential for trauma related to behavioral interventions. Case examples demonstrate how to apply a paradigm shift to clinical practice that seeks to accept and celebrate differences rather than mask them.

Animal Assisted Therapy in Communication Sciences and Disorders: The State of the Evidence
Presented by Sharon Antonucci, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9434Level: Introductory1 Hour
This is Part 1 of a two-part series. This course will review the evidence base for animal-assisted therapy (AAT), including discussion of areas where further research is needed, and will describe uses of AAT in speech-language treatment for cognitive and communication impairments including autism, dementia, traumatic brain injury, and aphasia.

Incorporating Animal-Assisted Therapy into Speech-Language Pathology Clinical Practice: An Overview
Presented by Sharon Antonucci, PhD, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #9436Level: Intermediate1 Hour
This is Part 2 of a two-part series. In this course, practical guidelines will be reviewed related to incorporating animal-assisted therapy into clinical practice. This overview will include information regarding training requirements for animal-handler teams, benefits/challenges of the SLP acting as a handler, and clinical decision-making, including goal-setting, when incorporating animals into speech-language pathology clinical care.

Guided Metaphors and Their Use in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Speech Disorders
Presented by Tim Mackesey, CCC-SLP, BRS-FD
Video
Course: #8741Level: Intermediate1 Hour
The integration of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and guided metaphors with behavioral models of SLP delivery is often vital to unleashing confidence and the courage to communicate. This course will demonstrate how stories can be used as metaphors to help improve outcomes for individuals with speech disorders.

Relationships between Language and Executive Functions: Planning and Regulating
Presented by Jill K. Fahy, MA, CCC-SLP
Video
Course: #8998Level: Intermediate1.5 Hour
This is Part 2 of a two-part series. This course will detail the relationship between language and executive functions (EFs), and the nature of EF deficits in children with language disorders. The use of inner speech and self-talk to regulate behaviors and efforts, and the use of complex syntax as a tool to reason, plan, predict, and solve will be discussed. Additionally, the course will describe the use of hands-on problem solving, Socratic questions, and discovery learning to elicit language-based EFs. (Part 1: Course 8993)